Skip to content
Permalink
a066bcbf03
Switch branches/tags

Name already in use

A tag already exists with the provided branch name. Many Git commands accept both tag and branch names, so creating this branch may cause unexpected behavior. Are you sure you want to create this branch?
Go to file
 
 
Cannot retrieve contributors at this time
622 lines (452 sloc) 51.8 KB
\chapter{Methodology}
Knowledge about how students conceptualize has a qualitative nature. For
qualitative research, methodology varies, but has standard parts: design of the
study, sources and their selection, data, the process of analysis, the interpretation,
and the approach to validation. Sample selection is recorded and reported
so that others may judge transferability to their own context.
The kinds of data in a qualitative study include interviews and documents.
Interviews are
the principle technique used by phenomenographical research.
%Documents can also be used.
Normal conduct of teaching can also provide data that can
be used, if in an anonymous, aggregate form. Both deductive and inductive
analysis can be carried out on these data.
The analysis produces a description of the situation under study.
This description may include a narrative, often called a thick and rich description, and also specific attributes, such as categories of findings and relationships among these categories.
\section{Design of the Study}
Information learned in tutoring and lecturing undergraduates inspired the research questions.
More specifically, questions asked by the students suggested that they were not learning enough about proof techniques to understand material that appeared later in the curriculum.
So, it seemed useful to discover what their ideas were, about proofs.
We used Bloom's taxonomy of the cognitive domain\cite{bloom1956taxonomy} to subdivide the domain in which we hope to find student ideas.
Correspondingly we created parts of the study: recognition and comprehension were grouped together into one part, application was a part of the study, and the third part included analysis, synthesis, and evaluation.
We chose a qualitative approach because
we seek to be able to describe the nature of the various
understandings achieved by the students, rather than the relative frequency
with which any particular understanding is obtained.
We chose a phenomenographic approach because it is aimed at identifying and expressing student understandings in a way that transforms these understandings into suggestions how to help them advance their learning.
We collected data about recognition and comprehension in interviews, and in group help sessions and during tutoring, and also with a written list of questions, and by incorporating observations of computer science classes.
We collected data about application on homeworks, and on practice and actual examinations.
We collected data about analysis in interviews.
We collected data about synthesis on homeworks, and on practice and actual examinations.
%The study was designed to observe undergraduate students as they progressed through the curriculum.
%Changes in conceptualizations of students as they progressed through the curriculum would be interesting if we could detect them.
%Consistent with a phenomenographic study, the principle data were interview transcripts.
We
conducted over 30 interviews.
The conceptualizations of undergraduate computing students were sought.
We incorporated into our design, a method of validation, called triangulation.
To check our findings, we also interviewed faculty and graduate students who had provided courses related to proof.
Our interview participants were sampled from a large public research-oriented university in the northeastern United States.
%this paragraph below is good.
Consistent with a grounded theory approach, we used interviews conducted early in the study to explore students' notions of proof, adapting to the
student preference for proof by mathematical induction and incorporating the use of recursive
algorithms.
We used interviews conducted later in the study to investigate questions that developed from analysis of earlier interviews.
We used exams to study errors in application of the pumping lemma for regular
languages.
We used homework
to observe student attempts at proofs, and
to observe student familiarity/facility
with different (specific) proof techniques: induction, construction, contradiction,
and what students think it takes to make an argument valid.
We used yet later interviews to discover whether students used proof techniques on their own, and how students ascertained whether circumstances were appropriate for the application of algorithms they knew, and how students ascertained certain properties of algorithms.
%\chapter{Design of the Study}
%This work is a qualitative study, the underlying philosophy is constructivist,
%the research perspective is phenomenography, as extended to variation theory and structural relevance,
%and the epistemological framework is
%social constructivism, in particular, that mental preparation is influential, readying students to learn from instructors and peer interaction, some material better than other material.
%a layered collection of intellectual disciplines,
%including
% complexity applied to cognitive neuroscience, and neurophysiology.
%At the highest level of integration, computer science and mathematics
% reside, supported by studies in memory and attention, including computational
%complexity applied to cognitive neuroscience, and neurophysiology.
% the focus is on determining what questions
%would be posed, in the process of continuous curriculum adaptation and improvement
%the meaning students are making of their specific educational experiences.
\section{Parts of the Study}
The parts of the study reflect the several research questions.
The parts of the study are organized taking inspiration from Bloom's Taxonomy of Cognitive Domain.\cite{bloom1956taxonomy}
The first part of the study was about recognition: what undergraduates think proof is, and comprehension: how they go about understanding them, and about structural relevance (part of phenomenography\cite{marton1997learning}: why students think proof is taught.
The second part was about application: How students attempt to apply proof.
The third part was about analysis, synthesis and evaluation: what students do when a situation might be well-addressed by proof.
This part informed us about any structure students used as they pursued proof-related activities, and about what students thought was required for an attempted proof to be valid.
This part also informed us about the comfort level students have about the use of proof, and the consequences students experience, as a result of their choices about application of proof.
These parts are summarized in Table \ref{parts}.
\begin{table}
\caption{Parts of the Study}
\begin{tabular}{|p{4cm}|p{10cm}|}\hline
Part & Purposes \\\hline \hline
recognition, & what proof is\\
comprehension and& how students approach understanding proofs\\
structural relevance & purpose of teaching proof\\\hline
application & how students apply proofs they have been taught\\ \hline
analysis, & use of structure\\
& how is validity attained\\
synthesis and & comfort level with proof\\
& student use of proof\\
& consequences of not applying proof\\
evaluation &\\ \hline
\end{tabular}
\label{parts}
\end{table}
\section{Population Studied}
The population of interest is undergraduate students in the computing disciplines.
In a phenomenographic study, it is desirable to sample widely to obtain as broad as possible a view of the multiple ways of experiencing a phenomenon within the population of interest.\cite{marton1997learning} which in turn cites Glaser and Strauss 1967\cite{glaser1968discovery} We studied undergraduate students who have taken computer science courses involving proof. Typically but not always, these are students majoring in computer science. Some of these undergraduate students are dual majors, in computer science and mathematics. We interviewed graduate students emphasizing those who have been teaching assistants for courses involving proofs. We interviewed faculty who have been taught courses involving proof. We have interviewed former students who have graduated from the department. We have interviewed undergraduates who transferred out of the department.
The demographics of the interviewed students is representative of the demographics of the computer science/engineering department.
Every student who signed a consent form was requested to schedule an interview, from an interval including 8AM to 9PM.
Every student who scheduled an interview was interviewed.
For the benefit of readers wondering to what extent the results might be transferable, demographics of some commonly seen properties of populations are provided:
Number of interviews transcribed:\\
Number of interview subjects:\\
Percentage by race:\\
Percentage by sex:\\
Percentage by age:\\
Percentage by language:\\
Percentage by domestic/international:\\
% \section{Chronology of the Design}
% The design of this study began while teaching Introduction to the Theory of
% Computing. While helping students learn the pumping lemma for regular
% languages, and trying to understand from where the several difficulties arose,
% I became curious about the bases of these difficulties. One example was that
% a student felt strongly that a variable, a letter, denoting repetitions in a mathematical
% formulation, could only stand for a single numeric value, rather than a
% domain. Subsequently I have learned that symbolization is a category identified
% by Harel and Sowder \cite{harel1998students}, for students of mathematics learning proofs. Our
% student is a vignette of our computer science student population harboring
% some of the same conceptualizations. As a consequence of this opinion, the
% student felt that showing that a mathematical formulation had a true value
% was equivalent to demonstrating a true value for a single example, rather than
% demonstrating a true value for a domain. Here we see evidence for the category
% Harel and Sowder \cite{harel1998students} call (is it inductive, perceptual?) where an example
% is thought to provide proof of a universal statement. Later, while helping students
% study the relationship between context free grammars and pushdown
% automata, I learned from the students that many of them did not find inductive
% proofs convincing. Subsequently I have learned that Harel and Sowder \cite{harel1998students} created
% a category called axiomatic reasoning. In axiomatic reasoning, students
% begin with accepted information, such as axioms and premises, and apply rules
% of inference to deduce the desired goal. This category had not always been
% reached by their students, similarly to ours. As will be seen, later interview data showed,
% some of our students learn to produce the artifact of a proof by mathematical
% induction by procedure. They learn the parts, and they supply the parts
% when asked, but are not themselves convinced. (McGowan and Tall report a similar situation.) This matches with two other
% categories created by Harel and Sowder \cite{harel1998students}, internalization and interiorization.
% Still later, when leading a course on ethical reasoning for issues related to computer
% science, I found that most of the students did not notice that methods
% of valid deductive argumentation were tools that they might apply to defend
% their opinions.
% Thus the idea of exploring the nature of the students' degrees of preparation
% for understanding and creating proofs appeared.
% First, interviews about proofs in general were conducted, with a broad interview
% script.
%The students almost all selected proofs by mathematical induction.
% During analysis of these data, a more elaborate interview script was developed,
% aiming at the ideas of domain, range, relation, mapping, function, the ideas of
% variable, as in programs and mathematical formulations, and abstraction.
% Some students emphasized that mathematical definitions are analogous to
% definitions in natural languages, and that mathematical discourse is carried
% out in the mathematical language created by these definitions.
% The capabilities for expression and care bestowed by these definitions invest
% mathematical reasoning with its persuasive power.
% Thus both the reasoning processes, using concepts and the clearly defined
% mathematical concepts together provide the ability of mathematical argumentation
% to be convincing. Students who appreciated this found it invigorating.
% Other students had different reactions to definitions. Thus, the role of definitions
% and language became another area of exploration.
% The difference between a domain and a single point in a domain can be seen as
% a level of abstraction. If something is true for a single point in a domain, but is
% also true for every single point in the domain, then the point can be seen as a
% generic particular point, representative of the domain. This concept of ability
% to represent is related to the idea of abstraction.
% We saw data in this study that affirmed the observations of others, that students
% do not always easily recognize the possibility of abstraction.
% \section{Sample Selection}
All participants were volunteers.
Volunteers were sought in all computer science undergraduate classes involving proofs, and also some that did not involve proofs, so that we could sample students at different stages in their undergraduate careers.
Graduate student volunteers were also sought. Most of the graduate student interviews were among teaching assistants in courses that taught and/or used proofs. We also included faculty of courses that involved proofs. Graduate student and faculty provided another perspective that was used as triangulation, a validation method in qualitative research.
% Students from the University of Connecticut who have taken or are taking the relevant courses were offered the opportunity to be interviewed.
The undergraduate students
who volunteered were mostly male, mostly traditionally aged students. Some students were domestic, and some international.
Some students were African-American,
some Asian, some Caucasian, some Latino/a, some with learning disabilities such as being diagnosed as on the autistic spectrum.
\subsection{Proofs by Mathematic Induction}
This part of the study contributes to recognition and comprehension of proof, and also to synthesis, yielding insight into consequences of student use (or not) of proof when the situation warrants.
The participants for the study of proof by mathematic induction
%We studied students who
were taking, or
% who
had recently taken, a course
on Discrete Systems required of all computer science, and computer science and
engineering students.
Volunteers were solicited from all students attending the Discrete Systems
courses.
Interviews of eleven students were transcribed for this study. Participants
included 2 women and 9 men. Two were international students, a third was a
recent immigrant.
%Every student who signed a consent form was requested to schedule an interview, from an interval including 8AM to 9PM.
%Every student who scheduled an interview was interviewed.
\subsection{Purpose of Proof}
This part of the study contributes the first part; it is about structural relevance.
Undergraduate students were sought for this study, because we wanted to know what students thought the purpose was while they were taking the undergraduate subjects.
\subsection{Proofs Using the Pumping Lemma for Regular Languages}
This part of the study contributes the second part; it is about how students apply proofs they have been taught.
The participants for the study of proofs using the pumping lemma for regular languages were
forty-two students, of whom thirty-four were men and eight women,
forty-one traditional aged,
%one former Marine somewhat older, one collegiate athlete (a
%woman),
there were three students having Latin-heritage surnames, 1/4 of the
students had Asian heritage, 2 had African heritage, and 8 were international
students. Each student individually took the final exam. A choice among
five questions was part of the final exam; one required applying the pumping
lemma. Half the students (21/42) selected this problem. These were 17 men
and 4 women. Three quarters of those (15/42) selecting the pumping lemma
got it wrong. These students, who chose the pumping lemma problem and
subsequently erred on it, form the population of our study.
\subsection{Student Use of Proof for Applicability of Algorithms}
This part of the study contributes the third part, about student use of proof.
The students participating in this part were mainly those having internships or summer jobs. This changed the ratio of domestic to international students, such that a greater proportion were domestic students. Also, the ratio of women to men students was affected, such that a greater proportion were male students.
\section{Data Collection}
Our corpus includes interview transcripts, homework, practice and real tests,
and observations from individual tutoring sessions, and group help sessions. %Interview transcripts were analyzed with thematic analysis.
Homework, and practice
and real tests, from several different classes were analyzed for proof attempts.
(Incidentally, data from multiple instructors was combined, and no use of information about any specific instructor was used.)
Data from individual tutoring sessions and group help sessions were also informative.
Aggregations of anonymous data were used.
\subsection{Interviews}
An application to the Institutional Review Board was approved, for the conduct of the interviews. The protocol numbers include H13-065 and H14-112.
%Consistent with phenomenographic studies, we wished to sample widely, so we sampled not only current students of courses involving proof, but also teaching assistants, faculty and former students associated with these courses. Some students are strictly CS/E majors, others are dual majors or minors in CS/E and math. Some students are not CS/E majors. Some former students are professionally employed in development, and others have left the major.
% All interview participants were volunteers.
The audio portion of all interviews was collected by electronic recorder and subsequently transferred to a password protected computer. From here the interviews were transcribed, and names were redacted. The redacted interview data were analyzed using the Saturate application.
\subsubsection{Student Conceptions of What Proofs Is}
Interviews were solicited in class by general announcement, and by email.
Interviews were conducted in person, using a voice recorder. No further
interview script, beyond these following few questions, was used. The interviews
began with a general invitation to discuss students' experience with and
thoughts on proofs from any time, such as high school, generally starting with
\begin{itemize}
\item ``Tell me anything that comes to your mind on the subject of using proofs,
creating proofs, things like that.''
\end{itemize}
and then following up with appropriate questions to get the students to elaborate
on their answers.
Additional questions from the script that were used when appropriate included
\begin{itemize}
\item ``Why do you think proofs are included in the computer science curriculum?'',
\item ``Do you like creating proofs?''
\end{itemize}
and, after proof by induction was discussed,
\begin{itemize}
\item “Do you see any relation between proof by induction and recursive algorithms?”.
\end{itemize}
Almost every student introduced and described proof by mathematic induction as experienced
in their current or recent class.
\subsection{Documents}
\subsubsection{Proofs Using the Pumping Lemma for Regular Languages}
The study was carried out on both real and practice exam documents. The interpretation was informed
by the events that occurred in the natural conduct of lectures,
help sessions and tutoring.
One method of assessing whether students understood the ease of application
of the pumping lemma to a language to be proved not regular was offering a
choice between using the Myhill-Nerode theorem with a strong hint or using
the pumping lemma. The pumping lemma problem, which could very easily
have been solved by application of the Myhill-Nerode theorem, especially with
the supplied hint, was designed, when tackled with the pumping lemma, to
require, for each possible segmentation, a different value of $i$ (the number of
repetitions) that would create a string outside of the language. The intent was
to separate students who understood the meaning of the equation's symbols,
and the equation itself, from those students engaged in a manipulation with at
most superficial understanding.
\subsection{Observations from Tutoring and Help Sessions}
These were recorded, at the conclusion of the help session or tutoring session, into notes for manuscripts under preparation at the time.
\section{Method of Analysis}
The phenomenographic approach to analysis has been written about by Marton and Booth\cite{marton1997learning}.
This method works on interview and other data, and aims to produce a set of categories with relationships among them. Moreover, these categories and relations are used to infer critical aspects, which are ideas that are critical for developing to a more advanced conceptualization from a less advanced conceptualization.
The process by which this transformation of data occurs has been further clarified by Marton and Booth\cite[p.103]{marton1997learning}, who have written that an analyst should apply "the principle of focusing on one aspect of the object and seeking its dimension of variation while holding other aspects frozen" is helpful.
One example of applying this principle is the analysis directed to the question of what students think about why proofs are taught in the curriculum. Using the terminology of Marton and Booth, "structural relevance", we consider structural relevance to be an aspect of proof in the curriculum. Students should learn about proof for reasons that are connected with other material in the curriculum. For example, proof by mathematic induction is relevant for understanding the explanation of why context free grammars generate the languages accepted by non-deterministic pushdown automata. We focus on the idea of the students' conceptions of why proof is taught. We look for a dimension of variation: some of the students' ideas about why proof is taught will contain more of the reason underlying the presence of proof in the curriculum. Using this single dimension we can sequence excerpts of student interview transcripts, student utterances, according to how little or much of this reason they recognize. This exercise is provided as an example in Table \ref{exemplar}.
% % % structural relevance
Excerpts of student transcripts were selected on the basis of being related to this question. A dimension of variation emerged from the data, such that the excerpts seemed readily organized along this dimension.
\begin{longtable}{|p{7cm}|p{8.5cm}|}\hline
\caption{Phenomenographic Analysis of Reasons for Teaching Proof}\label{exemplar}
\endfirsthead
\multicolumn{2}{c}%
{{\bfseries \tablename\ \thetable{} -- continued from previous page}} \\
\hline \multicolumn{1}{|c|}{\textbf{Category}} &
\multicolumn{1}{c|}{\textbf{Representative}} \\ \hline
\endhead
\hline \multicolumn{2}{|r|}{{Continued on next page}} \\ \hline
\endfoot
\hline \hline
\endlastfoot
% \begin{tabular}{|p{7cm}|p{8.5cm}|}
\hline
Category & Representative\\\hline\hline
some students do not see any point to proof&
They teach it to us because they were mathematicians and they like it.\\
& we didn't see ok why do i really have to know the proof of the theorem to do that right? We didn't see the point, because no one taught us the point, so, that's a very important part that was missing.\\\hline
some students think that it satisfies the curriculum goals, to be able to reproduce a previously taught proof, or follow a procedure to generate a proof, without being personally convinced&
I was able to get a full score, but I don't understand why a proof by induction is convincing\\\hline
Some students do not see a relationship between a problem and approach&
When I have to prove anything, I always start with proof by mathematic induction, that was the one they taught the most.\\\hline
Some students are surprised to discover that there is a relation between proof by induction and recursion&
I never noticed that before, but now that you mention it, I see that they are isomorphic.\\\hline
Some students see the relationship but do not use it&
Professor (redacted) would be really proud of me that I learned to understand proof by induction quite well. \ldots I understand how recursion matches induction, there's a base case, there's a way of proceeding. \ldots I just couldn't figure out how to program the merge-sort algorithm.\\\hline
some students think the only reason for studying proof is to understand proofs of, for example, resource utilization of known algorithms&
I would never consider writing a proof except on an assignment.\\
& I understand the proof of the lower bound on comparison sort. \ldots I understand the proof of the upper bound on searching in a binary search tree. \ldots If I had to prove something about termination on a search tree, I don't know how I would do that.\\
& I know that recursion has the same structure as proof by mathematical induction. \ldots If I had an algorithm with a recursive data structure like a tree, and I had to prove something like termination about it, I'm not sure what approach I would use, it would depend.\\\hline
Some students see that they could employ proof to explore whether an algorithm can be expected to solve a problem in a given context that includes bounds upon resources that are available for consumption. & mostly design the algorithm first, we had some expectation of what that complexity results would be and then we try to find an approach to prove.
%\end{tabular}
\end{longtable}
Marton and Booth\cite[p. 133]{marton1997learning} note that the phenomenographic method of analysis includes viewing excerpts of student utterances in specific perspectives. They advise "establish a perspective with boundaries, within which one seeks variation", and to remember to apply perspectives "that pertaining to the individual and that pertaining to the collective". So, when we establish a perspective with boundaries, we set a scope, allowing us to admit student text fragments relevant to that scope, filtering out other remarks. When we sequence or categorize the selected utterances, during which we will be comparing data from difference individuals, we must evaluate the utterances within the context of the interview from which they were obtained. For example, one student might be more prone to exaggeration than another. Also, one student may have more mathematical background than another.
Marton and Booth regard the learning objective as a collection of related aspects, with their relationships; we can observe that a component hierarchy can represent the aspects. Marton and Booth discuss the depth of understanding; we can observe that one consequence of depth of understanding is the development of a generalization/specialization hierarchy. Marton and Booth contrast situations with phenomena, such that phenomena are understandings and situations serve as relatively concrete examples of phenomena, as used in instruction and assessment.
We may search for evidence of recognition of aspects; they might be mentioned by learners. Marton and Booth have observed that in different context, different aspects shift between foreground (consciousness) and background.
Marton and Booth advise us to ``assume that what people say is logical from their point of view''\cite[p. 134]{marton1997learning}, citing Smedlund\cite{smedslund1970circular}
Marton and Booth \cite[p. 133]{marton1997learning} write that completion may be recognized by the achievement of a result, specifically the ability to identify a number of qualitatively different ways in which phenomenon has been experienced (not forgetting different methods of expression).
One approach we have taken, besides the single aspect oriented approach exemplified in Table \ref{exemplar}, is to apply basic inductive analysis and deductive qualitative analysis, including axial coding, with the phenomenographic paradigm in mind.
More specifically, when processing interview data, we transcribe the data, we transfer the transcribed and redacted\footnote{Names of people are removed.} data to a website-based tool named Saturate. We have used both the current Saturate application and the previous version. The previous version has features we prefer to the more recent version.
We use the Saturate application to select contiguous fragments of text the capture meaning in our judgment. Each selected fragment is labeled. These labels, which are also called codes, can be reused, thus collecting together multiple fragments, as synonymous. A process, called constant comparison, begins at this level of aggregation of the data. A code, representing the synonymous fragments, is chosen, either from among the fragments or not. The fragments sharing a code are compared with one another, to ascertain whether the group with that code is internally cohesive, to such a degree that fragments in any one group are relatively distinct from fragments in other groups. A summary description (called a memo) of each code is written, and fragments are checked for compatibility with the code's description.
Data were analyzed using a modified version of thematic analysis, which is
in turn a form of basic inductive analysis.\cite{Merriam2002,Merriam2009,braun2006using,fereday2008demonstrating,boyatzis1998transforming} Using thematic analysis, we
read texts, including transcripts, looked for “units of meaning”, and extracted
these phrases. Deductive categorization began with defined categories, and
sorted data into them. Inductive categorization “learned” the categories, in
the sense of machine learning, which is to say, the categories were determined
from the data, as features and relationships found among the data suggested
more and less closely related elements of the data. A check on the development
of categories compared the categories with the collection of units of meaning.
Each category was named by either an actual unit of meaning (obtained during
open coding\footnote{Open coding is so-called because it occurs at a time when the analyst is the most open-minded about what the meanings being found in the data might be. \cite[p. 178]{merriam2009qualitative}}) or a synonym (developed to capture the essence of the category).
A memo was written to capture the summary meaning of the category.
Then, with a set of codes, we again perform grouping. This time we group codes into categories. Each category is reviewed to check whether the codes contained are relatively cohesive within a category, and relatively distinct from codes in other categories. A memo is written for each category.
Categories at this point in the analysis are also called initial themes. Themes are used in the process of axial coding. The word axial refers to the hub and spoke placement of the categories, as, one at a time, each category takes its place as a hub in a diagram, with one spoke for each of the other categories, that are ranged around the hub in a circle. Each spoke is labeled with a pairwise relationship between categories. After the relationships have been inferred by considering each pair of categories, the relationships are themselves compared. Participation in multiple strong relationships distinguishes a category, promoting it to a (what's the adjective, something like principle) theme.
Next a process called axial coding, found in the literature on grounded theory,
\cite{strauss1990basics,kendall1999axial,glaser2008conceptualization} was applied. This process considered each category in turn as a central
hub; attention focused on pairwise relations between that central category
with each of the others. The strength and character of the posited relationship
between each pair of categories was assessed. On the basis of the relationships
characterized in this exercise, the categories with the strongest interesting relationships
were promoted to main themes.
Attending to the phenomenographic paradigm, we seek dimensions of variation. These are delineated by the appearance of multiple categories that are usually related to each other by including more aspects (component parts) of an idea that is a learning objective.
The basic inductive analysis with axial coding described above should make more evident relationships in the data that are of the nature of a dimension of variation. Thus we see that basic inductive analysis with axial coding is compatible with phenomengraphic analysis, in that it can be directed towards achieving the goals of phenomenographic analysis insofar as one or more dimensions of variation can emerge.
Phenomenographic analysis proceeds beyond the identification of categories and relationships to infer critical aspects. These are differences between related categories, such that discernment by students of the ideas differentiating those two related categories are thought necessary for the students' depth of understanding to develop into the more inclusive category.
A diagram showing the main
themes and their relationships, qualified by the other, subsidiary themes and
the relationships between the subsidiary and main themes was prepared to
present the findings. Using the process of constant comparison, the structure
of these relationships was reviewed in the light of the meanings of the categories.
A memo was written about each relationship in the diagram, referring
to the meaning of the categories and declaring the meaning of the relationship.
A narrative was written to capture the content of the diagram. Using the
process of constant comparison, the narrative was reviewed to see whether it
captured the sense of the diagram. Units of meaning were compared with the
narrative and their original context, to see whether the narrative seemed to
capture the meaning. The products of the analysis were the narrative and the
diagram.
Using Marton's overriding categorization of task and objective, we can consider that some students do not know, at least when they are studying CSE2500, that they need to be able to understand some proofs, to be good developers. Therefore, they can logically approach the study of proof in CSE2500 as a task, having some facts that they must memorize. Other students, including those with dual majors in math, wish to improve themselves by improving their ability to couch arguments in mathematical terms and both ascertain facts for themselves and convince others. Students who are aware that there are computer-science related purposes for proof, for example, in the study of algorithms they will be using proofs to understand resource consumption, will recognize the study as having the objective to improve themselves vis-a-vis dealing with proof.
Because the relationships are expected to form a partial order, corresponding to set inclusion of subsets of the complete (with respect to the objective of teaching) understanding of the information being taught, we can say relationships \textit{between} categories.
The set inclusion relationship can be that a deeper understanding includes a more superficial understanding.
It may also be that a deeper understanding qualifies a more superficial understanding, such as being applicable in a restricted domain. Thus, understanding of a liquid as being divisible to any degree can be qualified as to scale such as macroscopic, microscopic and so on.
In a phenomenographic study, this partial order is referred to as a hierarchical order.
The objective of teaching, as will be in some parts of this study, the components of proof, may have many parts, called, in phenomenology, internal structure. The granularity of the subdivision of the objective of teaching results in the number of parts in the internal structure. If we let $n$ denote the number of parts obtained with a specific granularity, we see that the number of subsets will be $2^n$, which will be inconveniently large unless the granularity is sufficiently coarse. Thus, we choose a granularity resulting in approximately 4 elements of internal structure.
Thus, when the teaching objective is, what is a proof, we may limit the granularity, such that the internal structure of a proof is, for example,
\begin{enumerate}
\item that particular statement which is to be proved
\item axioms, premises, suppositions, cases
\item other statements
\item warrants (rules of inference)
\end{enumerate}
We might choose to pursue finer granularity in some cases, for example, we might pursue "What is a statement?", because instructors have found that not all students arriving in CSE3502 have the same depth of understanding of statement, and some do not have sufficiently deep understanding of "statement" to be able to comprehend a proof.
Marton and Booth\cite[p. 22]{marton1997learning} call our attention to learners directing their attention to the sign vs. to the signified. With proofs, Polya \cite{} has mentioned a procedural approach to executing a proof, without understanding, as have Harel and Sowder \cite{harel1998students}, and Tall\cite{tall2001symbols}. Weber and Alcock\cite{weber2004semantic} have observed and described students omitting understanding of warrants in proofs. In each of these cases, the sign is provided, but the signified is at best incompletely understood.
Analysis can usefully illuminate learning processes, taking note of the temporal domain\cite{marton1997learning}. This has been used by Booth in her analysis of how students understand the process of programming\cite{marton1997learning}.
[p. 136]\cite{marton1997learning} important to be looking whether conceptualizations appear in a certain case, in a certain period of time (such as, when see proofs again in 3500, 3502, are they recognized as proofs, some no some yes, are they helpful as proofs, or troublesome, some helpful, some not, "never did get that").
\subsection{Analysis of Interviews}
Items excerpted from interviews for analysis should be analyzed in the context of the specific interview and also in the context of the ensemble.\cite{marton1997learning}.
Data were analyzed multiple ways. Both an orthodox phenomenographic analysis, and a modified thematic analysis were carried out.
\subsubsection{Orthodox Phenomenographic Analysis}
In the orthodox phenomenographic analysis of interviews, the transcriptions are printed, and text fragments corresponding to units of meaning are cut out (as, with scissors). These pieces are then grouped (making copies if necessary) according to a sense of similarity. During a stage in the process, categories are learned, as researchers sense of features that distinguish categories evolves. During this stage, text fragments are moved from one category to another. After this category development phase, researchers, look into each category, to recognize and describe each category. Subsequently the perspective is shifted so that relations between categories are sought. Thus the categories are arranged relative to one another, and pairwise relations, where they exist, are identified and described. This produces a graph. From the graph, critical features of the learning objective are inferred.
\subsubsection{Modified Thematic Analysis}
Data were analyzed using a modified version of thematic analysis, which is
in turn a form of basic inductive analysis.\cite{Merriam2002,Merriam2009,braun2006using,fereday2008demonstrating,boyatzis1998transforming} Using thematic analysis, we
read texts, including transcripts, looked for “units of meaning”, and extracted
these phrases. Deductive categorization began with defined categories, and
sorted data into them. Inductive categorization “learned” the categories, in
the sense of machine learning, which is to say, the categories were determined
from the data, as features and relationships found among the data suggested
more and less closely related elements of the data. A check on the development
of categories compared the categories with the collection of units of meaning.
Each category was named by either an actual unit of meaning (obtained during
open coding) or a synonym (developed to capture the essence of the category).
A memo was written to capture the summary meaning of the category.
Next a process called axial coding, found in the literature on grounded theory,
\cite{strauss1990basics,kendall1999axial,glaser2008conceptualization} was applied. This process considered each category in turn as a central
hub; attention focused on pairwise relations between that central category
with each of the others. The strength and character of the posited relationship
between each pair of categories was assessed. On the basis of the relationships
characterized in this exercise, the categories with the strongest interesting relationships
were promoted to main themes. A diagram showing the main
themes and their relationships, qualified by the other, subsidiary themes and
the relationships between the subsidiary and main themes was prepared to
present the findings. Using the process of constant comparison, the structure
of these relationships was reviewed in the light of the meanings of the categories.
A memo was written about each relationship in the diagram, referring
to the meaning of the categories and declaring the meaning of the relationship.
A narrative was written to capture the content of the diagram. Using the
process of constant comparison, the narrative was reviewed to see whether it
captured the sense of the diagram. Units of meaning were compared with the
narrative and their original context, to see whether the narrative seemed to
capture the meaning. The products of the analysis were the narrative and the
diagram.
\subsection{Analysis of Help Session and Tutoring}
Help sessions were scheduled weekly; attendance was optional. Typically six to twelve students would participate.
Originally these were called help sessions, but the demographics of the attendees did not represent the enrolled students.
Subsequently the name was changed to consultation sessions.
This change had the desired effect, that the population attending better reflected the enrolled students.
At these sessions, students would raise topics about which they had questions.
Frequently the student would be requested to work at the white board, and leading questions were asked, and problems of very small size were posed, to urge the student along the right path of development of a solution.
Occasionally these suggested paths were met with resistance from the students, which is to say, misunderstandings were encountered and discussed.
Ideas mentioned in these discussions that were relevant to manuscripts in process at the time were noted, anonymized, into the manuscripts.
The idea that a variable used in the pumping lemma could take on only one value, as if it were a single root of linear equation, rather than representing the domain of values possible for strings in a language, was proposed by a student in a help session. The context was a student proposition that examples were sufficient for proofs of universal statements, because the the variable in the pumping lemma could take on only a single value.
some students, who do know that any statement must and can, be
either true or false, thought implications must be true.
Due to attention being focused on interacting with students in the normal course of teaching, these field notes are incomplete.
One use of such data is that they can give evidence that categories of conceptualization of proof already created in the mathematics literature can be found also in computer science students. This is similar to a deductive rather than inductive process, in that we are aware of the categories created by Harel and Sowder\cite{harel1998students} and by Tall\cite{Tall?} and student utterances that seem well matched to those categories draw our attention to those categories, validating them for students of computer science.
% The study proceeded using prior interview experiences to suggest further investigation.
% Originally asking about proofs and what they were for, we received answers about proof by induction and found out not all students contemplate why the curriculum contains what it does.
% (reference Guzdial on students trusting that whatever curriculum they take, is very likely to qualify them for a job).
%When providing leading questions about why, interview data indicated that not all students apply proof techniques with which they have been successful.
%So, when application of proofs developed as a part of the study.
%Consequently, what clues there may be that prompt recall of proof and proof techniques to application to a problem became part of the study, involving structural relevance.
%Generalization is related to the presence of one situation evolving a response that was learned in the context of a different situation prompted the view of teacher teaching from context of generalization hierarchy present and situation as example and homework situation as another example.
%What opportunities to foster generalization do students notice?
% \subsection{Within What is a Proof?}
%The study was devoted to proofs, a subject that can be subdivided.
%Part of the study was aimed at the idea of domain, directed at the concept that
%though a variable could identify a scalar, it might also represent a set.
%Part of the study was aimed at the activity of abstraction, because some students
%exhibited the ability to operate at one level of abstraction, not necessarily a
%concrete level, yet the ability to traverse between that level of abstraction and
%a concrete level seemed to be absent. Other students claimed to be able to
%understand concrete examples with ease, but to encounter difficulty when
%short variable names were used within the same logical argument.
%\subsection{Order of Exploration}
%The order of exploration was data driven, thus the material was sought sometimes
%in reverse order of the curriculum, almost as if seeking bedrock by starting
%at a surface, and working downwards.
\subsection{Application of Phenomenographic Analysis to Why We Teach Proof}
The analysis for the research question "What do students think proof is for?", which was approached as "Why do you think we teach proof?" exemplifies a phenomenographic approach. One aspect of the phenomenon of proof is its utility. We set the scope of our perspective to be specific to usefulness. We selected student verbal productions related to the use of proof. We considered them in the context of their own interview, and we compared them to data from other interviews on the same theme.
We applied phenomenographic analysis by focusing on the aspect of relevance of proof for learning computer science and practicing as a software developer. In this case we had already identified the dimension of variation to be the depth of understanding of why we teach proof. Thus we could select fragments of student utterances and rank them according to depth of understanding. We then presented them in a sequence by rank.
\section{Method of Addressing Validation}
Triangulation is a technique for increasing the confidence that the results of analysis are reliable.
In this study we applied triangulation in several ways.
We interviewed faculty teaching the courses involving proofs. We interviewed TAs assisting in the courses involving proofs. The students in these courses are from our same population. To get an idea of the background preparation of our students, we substitute taught geometry and algebra II classes in a high school. The high school population was quite similar to our university population, but differed by consisting almost entirely of domestic students, studying in their first language, and by having a larger percentage of women students, and of declared transgender students. Though the community served by this high school is diverse over socio-economic status, this component of diversity is probably greater in our university population.
Consistency with the work of other researchers is a check on the validity of an analysis.
In this study we compared our results with those achieved by some other researchers in computer science education and also by some researchers in the mathematics education community.
Checking possible interpretations is a technique that may aid in increasing confidence in validity.
We prepared a list of questions that was addressed by several faculty and several students, that began an examination of the role of specific representation styles (mathematical notation, figures and pseudocode) for proof related problem statements.
\section{Method of Presentation of Results}
The product of analysis in a phenomenographic study is a set of categories, and relationships \textit{among} them. These categories and relationships are often depicted in a graph. This product may be accompanied by a "thick and rich" narrative description of the categories and relationships. This narrative must be consistent with the individual text fragments, excerpts from transcriptions, field notes or documents obtained for the study.
Marton and Booth\cite[p. 135]{marton1997learning} state "in the late stages of analysis, our researcher [has] a sharply structured object of research, with clearly related faces, rich in meaning. She is able to bring into focus now one aspect, now another; she is able to see how they fit together like pieces of a multidimensional jigsaw puzzle; she is able to turn it around and see it against the background of the different situations that it now transcends."
This tells us that the narrative should describe the categories of composition hierarchies found in the students' understandings. The faces or facets of the learning object have their importance and relationships as envisioned by the teacher. The students' conceptualizations may be less complete, contain superfluous items, and differ as to the relationships of the parts, especially by lacking profundity in understanding of relationships.